World's first medical networking and resource portal

News & Highlights
Please make use of the search function to browse preferred content
Medical News & Updates
Mar 20
Exercise lessens and watching TV ups effects of genes on obesity
Increasing leisure-time activity and reducing sedentary behaviors such as watching television will both independently attenuate genetic predisposition to obesity, a new study reveals [1].

"We cannot change genes, but we can do something to change the influence of genes, by doing some exercise and by reducing our sedentary behavior, primarily by watching television less," lead author Dr Qibin Qi (Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA) told heartwire. Qi presented the results of the research today at EPI|NPAM 2012, the Epidemiology and Prevention/Nutrition, Physical Activity and Metabolism Scientific Sessions 2012.

Qi says he and his team's work is the first to study the modifying effect of TV watching, "which is the most common sedentary behavior," on genetic predisposition to obesity based on a risk score. They also examined the effects of physical exercise on this score.

Because they found independent effects of the two behaviors, Qi says he "wants to emphasize the importance of both." And he says those who increased physical activity didn't necessarily reduce TV viewing time. So an additional goal is to try to get people to exercise while watching TV, "at least stand up and move around," he suggests.

Activity attenuates effect size for BMI; TV viewing increases it

Qi said statistics show that people in Europe and the US watch, on average, four to five hours of TV per day. It is already known that the greater the daily duration of television viewing, the higher the risk of obesity and related disease, independent of physical activity, he noted.

He and his colleagues set out to examine whether leisure-time TV watching and physical activity modify the genetic predisposition to elevated adiposity in women and men, using data from the Nurses' Health Study (7740 women with genotyping data) and the Health Professionals Follow-up Study (4564 men with genotyping data).

They assessed body-mass index (BMI) and average weekly time spent watching TV and performing physical activity, both self-reported and validated (with an r=0.97 between reported BMI and measured BMI).

Qi explained that most previous studies on gene-lifestyle interaction and obesity have largely focused on a single locus, the fat mass and obesity-associated gene (FTO). Instead, he and his colleagues calculated a genetic predisposition score. "The novel thing we did was include 32 genes, including FTO, and we created a score that estimates the overall genetic predisposition to obesity," where each point score corresponded to each BMI-increasing allele, he noted.

The results from women and men were pooled by meta-analysis.

Overall, each BMI-increasing allele was associated with an increase of 0.13 kg/m2 in BMI. The effect size for BMI in those in the highest physical-activity quintile was attenuated compared with that in individuals in the lowest physical-activity quintile (0.08 kg/m2 vs 0.15 kg/m2; p for interaction <0.001).

In contrast, the genetic effect on BMI was more pronounced in people who spent >40 hours watching TV than in those who spent 0 to one hour per week (0.34 kg/m2 vs 0.08 kg/m2; p for interaction=0.001).

One-hour-a-day walk cuts difference between max and min score by 50%

Qi and colleagues found that each 4-METs/day increment in physical activity-equivalent to one hour per day of brisk walking-was associated with a 0.06-kg/m2 reduction in BMI (approximately 46% of the main effect of each additional BMI-increasing allele), while each two-hour/day increment in TV watching was associated with a 0.03-kg/m2 increase in BMI (23% of the main effect).

"We estimated that the difference in BMI (4 kg/m2, equivalent to 11.6 kg in body weight for a person 1.7 m tall) between individuals with a genetic predisposition score of 13 (minimum) and those with a score of 43 (maximum) could be reduced by half (2.1 kg/m2, 6.1 kg in weight) by one hour per day of brisk walking or increased by 25% by two hours per day of TV watching," they say.

"Our data suggest that both increasing exercise levels and reducing sedentary behaviors, especially TV watching, independently may mitigate genetic disposition to increased BMI."

Qi acknowledges, however, that genetic testing is not yet widespread, so in the meantime, doctors and the public should use family history of obesity to guide them. "Maybe ask about relatives who are obese, to see if there is a high genetic risk," he advises.

Mar 20
Study: How Information Is Presented Online Can Affect Self-Diagnoses
The way that health information is presented online -- particularly the order in which symptoms are displayed -- can affect how patients diagnose themselves, according to a study published in the journal Psychological Science, Newsroom America reports (Newsroom America, 3/16).

Study Details

For the study, researchers from Arizona State University, the University of California-Irvine, Ono Academic College and the University of Warwick conducted two experiments.

In the first experiment, researchers presented students with lists of symptoms for a fictional type of cancer. The students were asked to check off the symptoms they had experienced and rate their likelihood of having the cancer.

The students were divided into three groups, which received either:

A list of three general symptoms followed by three specific ones;
A list of three specific symptoms followed by three general ones; or
A list alternating between general and specific symptoms.

In the second experiment, researchers presented the students with lists of either six or 12 symptoms for meningioma, a real type of cancer. The students were divided into groups, which received one of the three types of symptom lists used in the first experiment (Association for Psychological Science release, 3/15).

Key Findings

In the experiment that looked at symptoms for the fictional type of cancer, participants who had received the list alternating between specific and general symptoms were less likely than participants receiving the other two types of lists to say that they could have cancer (Chan, "Healthy Living," Huffington Post, 3/18).

Researchers wrote that when participants check off several symptoms in a row, "they perceive a higher personal risk of having that illness" (Newsroom America, 3/16).

In the experiment that looked at symptoms for the real cancer, researchers found that participants were less likely to think that having several symptoms in a row indicated that they could have cancer if they received the list of 12 symptoms instead of the list of six ("Healthy Living," Huffington Post, 3/18).

Researchers wrote that the longer list of symptoms allowed several boxes to be left unchecked, which helped participants feel reassured that they did not have cancer (APS release, 3/15).

Implications

Virginia Kwan -- a psychologist and the lead researcher of the study -- said the findings could be useful for public health education. She said that health officials could encourage people to seek health screenings sooner by grouping common and mild symptoms together when describing a disease

Mar 19
Cough syrup addiction: FDCA cracks down on city chemists
To prevent addicts from consuming branded syrups containing 'habit formatting' drug - codeine, the state Food and Drug Control Administration (FDCA) has suspended licences of nearly 15 chemists in Ahmedabad over the last one week.

The department has warned the chemists not to provide such syrups to people without a prescription from a registered medical practitioner.

FDCA commissioner H G Koshia said codeine is an element which is extracted from opium and which has delirious impact on human body. He said that recently the department's intelligence wing had conducted investigation in various areas like Sabarmati, Jamalpur, Behrampura, Odhav and Maninagar. And there they found many chemists selling cough syrups having codeine without mandatory prescription from a registered medical practitioner.

"Syrups having codeine in it are drugs of Schedule 'H', for which the doctor's prescription is must. But many people get addicted to it as many chemists sell it in open without fulfilling the statutory condition. So, we've carried out the exercise to prevent this," said Koshia.

According to Koshia, the department has also freezed around 10 lakh bottles of syrups containing codeine, worth Rs 4 crore, of different drug manufacturing companies.

He said under the provisions of the Drugs and Cosmetics Act, it is mandatory for drug manufacturers to prominently put the generic name of the respective drugs on their product. "But these companies are found manufacturing syrups without putting the generic name prominently on the bottles. So, we have prohibited their 10 lakh bottles and issued them notice," Koshia said.
He said the exercise was launched after discovering that Corex - a cough syrup brand - was found as one of the top-10 drugs in demand in the country in an authorised survey.

"Corex is a cough syrup and Gujarat is a dry state. So, it can be presumed that there won't be too many patients of cough. However, our intelligence officers have come to a conclusion that people are consuming it even without the illness due to the habit formatting codeine in it," Koshia said.

Mar 19
Poor literacy 'increases early death risk for elderly'
They are more than twice as likely to die within five years as adults with no literacy problems, it was found.

The University College London study tested nearly 8,000 adults on their understanding of aspirin instructions.

The Patients Association said patients should help draft information leaflets so they are "relevant and clear".

The researchers, from the department of epidemiology and public health at University College London, said that limited or low health literacy among older people has implications for the design and delivery of health services for this section of the population.

Using a short test containing four questions, based on instructions similar to those found on a packet of aspirin, the researchers assessed the participants' ability to read and understand the information.

They found that 67.5% had high health literacy (achieved the maximum score), 20% were classed as medium (made one error) and 12.5% had low health literacy (got two, one or no questions correct).

In the study, almost half of the adults aged over 80 could not correctly answer all four questions, compared to one-quarter of the adults aged 60 or less.
'Worrying'

The study followed the participants for an average of five years after the test.

During this time, 6.1% (321) died in the high health literacy category, 9% (143) in the medium category and 16% (157) in the low literacy category.

The researchers said that lower health literacy was linked to a higher prevalence of depressive symptoms, physical limitations and chronic diseases, such as heart disease, diabetes, stroke and asthma - but higher health literacy scores were linked to stronger cognitive abilities, including verbal fluency and working memory, they said.

When researchers adjusted for factors such as wealth, education, income, ethnicity and basic health, the link between low health literacy and mortality risk reduced, "but remained significant", the study said.

Katherine Murphy, chief executive of the Patients' Association said it was worrying that so many people struggle to understand health-related information.

"How are patients expected to make informed decisions if they do not fully understand the information being given to them?

"Patients must be involved in the development of these information leaflets from the outset to make sure they provide relevant and clear information."

Michelle Mitchell, director general of Age UK, said health professionals have a responsibility to give patients information that makes sense.

"That way, patients will be better empowered to manage their own health conditions and make the positive lifestyle choices that lead to better health and wellbeing."

Mar 17
Parents with obese children can help their kids drop pounds by losing weight themselves: study
Parents with obese children can help their kids lose weight by dropping pounds themselves, according to a new study.

More than restocking the fridge or parenting styles, researchers from California found that a parent's own weight is the most influential factor and key contributor to the successful weight loss of an obese child.

"Parents are the most significant people in a child's environment, serving as the first and most important teachers," said study co-author Kerri Boutelle of the University of California, San Diego in a statement. "They play a significant role in any weight-loss program for children, and this study confirms the importance of their example in establishing healthy eating and exercise behaviors for their kids."

For their study, researchers examined the effectiveness of three types of parenting skills among 142 families. Kids ranged between the ages of 8 and 12 and were either overweight or obese.

Their conclusion? Parents serving as role models and shed weight themselves proved to be the most inspiring and motivational method for their own children -- more than making changes to the home food environment or enrolling kids in physical activities, researchers said.

But the latest parent-child obesity study runs counter to another analysis published in 2010 in which researchers concluded that parental influence on their child's eating behavior is limited.

Mar 17
Eyesight 'clue' to mental decline
Looking at the back of the eye may offer an insight into the health of someone's brain, according to the US researchers.

A small study, published in the journal Neurology, linked damage to the retina with declining brain function.

They believe issues with the blood supply may be damaging both the eye and the brain.

Alzheimer's Research UK said the study showed the importance of a healthy heart.

The eye condition the researchers were looking at was retinopathy, which is common in patients with Type 2 diabetes or high blood pressure. Damage to the retina can eventually lead to blindness.

Scientists followed 511 women, who were 65 or older, for a decade. Some 39 were diagnosed with retinopathy.

Those with the eye condition tended to have lower scores in tests of brain function, including memory and abstract reasoning exams.

Brain scans also showed up more areas of damaged brain tissue, ischemic lesions, in those with retinopathy.

Dr Mary Haan, from the University of California, San Francisco, said: "Problems with the tiny blood vessels in the eye may be a sign that there are also problems with the blood vessels in the brain that can lead to cognitive problems.

"This could be very useful if simple eye screening could give us an early indication that people might be at risk of problems with their brain health and functioning."

There was only a small number of patients with retinopathy in the study. Much larger studies would be needed to see if the findings could be used as a clinical test for declining brain function.

While there was no suggestion of dementia in the patients, brain decline can be an early sign of the disease.

Dr Simon Ridley, head of research at Alzheimer's Research UK, said: "Accurate early detection of the cognitive decline that can be associated with dementia could unlock our ability to treat it.

"This small study offers clues for another possible route doctors could consider when monitoring for the signs of cognitive decline.

"The study adds to mounting evidence linking vascular health to cognitive decline, and underlines the importance of looking after our hearts. It will be useful to see whether the people in this study went on to develop dementia."

Mar 16
Women fed up with their jobs are 'more likely to turn to comfort eating'
If you struggle to curb your chocolate cravings or can't resist one more biscuit, forget working on your willpower. It could be time to look for another job.

Women who are over-worked or bored with their career are more likely to comfort eat, a Finnish study has found.

Fed-up females seek solace in emotional eating and 'uncontrolled eating' or in other words, not stopping until the last biscuit has gone from the packet.

The study, reported in the American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, found women who reported work burnout were more likely to develop an 'emotional' eating habit.

Study leader Nina Nevanpera, of the Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, said: 'Those experiencing burnout may be more vulnerable to emotional eating and uncontrolled eating and have a hindered ability to make changes in their eating behavior.'

'We recommend that burnout should be treated first and that burnout and eating behavior should be evaluated in obesity treatment.'

The findings are based on 230 women aged 30 to 55 who were part of a clinical trial looking at healthy lifestyle changes. All were employed, and at the start of the trial they completed surveys on job burnout and eating habits.

Overall, 22 per cent of the women had some degree of work burnout. As a group, they scored higher on measures of emotional eating and uncontrolled eating.

On top of that, women who did not have job burnout at the study's start tended to cut down on uncontrolled eating over one year. But, on average, the burnout group failed to make that change.

'Work permeates our lives,' said Sherry Pagoto, from the University of Massachusetts Medical School, who was not involved in the study.

'People may be in a job where they're unhappy, or a marriage where they're unhappy, and eating can become one of the few pleasures in their lives.'

There was no obvious effect of burnout on the women's weight, however. At the outset, half of the women reporting work burnout were normal weight - compared with a third of women reporting no burnout.

One reason might be education, Nevanpera said. Women with work burnout generally had a higher education level, and education, in turn, was linked to lower weight.

Still, she added, emotional eating is a potential risk factor for becoming heavy in the future. And it's not particularly healthy, since stressed-out people are more likely to reach for chocolate or fast food than an apple.

Professor Pagoto agreed that addressing sources of stress in general was important, adding that big stress triggers in life may make it difficult to lose weight and keep it off.

When people are not overweight, emotional eating is still not a good idea, she said.

'"It's reinforcing a habit that's not healthy.'

Mar 16
Are we programmed to be fat in the womb?
If the extra pounds you are saddled with don`t go away no matter what you do, the reason maybe your mother.

Research suggests a mother`s lifestyle can affect the health of the unborn and programme his or her DNA in the womb.

Her diet, the way she exercises, whether she smokes or drinks alcohol and even which pollutants she is exposed to can alter the DNA of her unborn child, the journal Public Library of Science Medicine reports.

The latest evidence for the theory comes from researchers at Newcastle University who analysed the genes of children aged between nine and 11 years, according to the Daily Mail.
They specifically looked for genes that had the same letters but behaved differently in children who were overweight and those who were slim. Blood samples stored since the children`s births were then analysed.

It showed that in many cases, overweight youngsters displayed different chemical marks on their DNA at birth.

Caroline Relton, who led the research, said: "This suggests that our DNA could be marked before birth and these marks could predict our later body composition."

Mar 15
Just 60 secs of combat can seriously impair memory
A new study of police officers has found that just 60 seconds of all out physical exertion in a threatening situation can seriously damage the memories of those involved for many details of the incident.

Police officers, witnesses and victims of crime suffer loss of memory, recognition and awareness of their environment if they have had to use bursts of physical energy in a combative encounter, according to scientists.


Researchers, led by Dr Lorraine Hope of the University of Portsmouth, found that less than 60 seconds of all out exertion, as might happen when an officer is forced to chase-down a fleeing suspect or engage in a physical battle with a resistant criminal, can seriously impair their ability to remember details of the incident or even identify the person who was involved.

Even officers in top condition are not immune to the rapid drain of physical prowess and cognitive faculties resulting from sustained hand-to-hand combat.

The findings are a stark warning to police officers, police chiefs and the courts, according to Dr Hope, a Reader in applied cognitive psychology of the university's Department of Psychology.

"Police officers are often expected to remember in detail who said what and how many blows were received or given in the midst of physical struggle or shortly afterwards. The results of our tests indicate it may be very difficult for them to do this," she said.

"As exhaustion takes over, cognitive resources tend to diminish. The ability to fully shift attention is inhibited, so even potentially relevant information might not be attended to. Ultimately, memory is determined by what we can process and attend to.

"The legal system puts a great deal of emphasis on witness accounts, particularly those of professional witnesses like police officers. Investigators and courts need to understand that an officer who cannot provide details about an encounter where physical exertion has played a role is not necessarily being deceptive or uncooperative. An officer's memory errors or omissions after an intense physical struggle should not unjustly affect his or her credibility," she noted.

Researchers recruited 52 police officers (42 males, 10 females) in Winnipeg, Canada, with an average of eight years on the job. All officers were fit and healthy and engaged in regular exercise.

During an initial briefing, the officers were given background information about a recent spate of armed robberies in the city. The briefing included details of how the robberies were conducted and witness descriptions of the perpetrators.

Half of the officers then engaged in a full force physical attack on a 300lb hanging water bag and the others (a control group) were assigned as observers.

The next part of the test required the officers to approach a trailer that a "known criminal" was suspected of occupying. On entering the trailer, the officer found themselves in a realistic living area where a number of weapons, including an M16 carbine, a revolver, a sawn off shotgun and a large kitchen knife were visible.

After a short delay, the "target individual" emerged from another room and shouted aggressively at the officer to get out of his property. The individual was not armed, but several of the weapons were within easy reach.

Dr Hope found those who had been asked to exert themselves physically remembered less about the target individual and made more recall errors compared to the control group of observers. The officers who had been exerted also recalled less about the initial briefing information and what they did report was less accurate. They also reported less about an individual they encountered incidentally while en route to the trailer.

While more than 90 per cent of non exerted observers were able to recall at least one descriptive item about him, barely one-third of exerted officers remembered seeing him at all.

Everyone remembered seeing the angry suspect in the trailer, but non exerted observers provided a significantly more detailed description of him and made half as many errors in recall as those who were exhausted.These observers were also twice as likely to correctly identify the suspect from a line up.

However, another striking aspect of the findings showed that exerted officers were able to register threat cues in the environment to the same degree at non exerted officers.

These new findings reveal that although exerted officers were able to pay attention to the threatening aspects of the scene, their ability to then process other aspects of the interaction was affected.

As a result of this, some information may only have been processed weakly or not at all resulting in an impaired memory for many details of the encounter.

Mar 15
Obesity caused by DNA during birth?
A question staring hard at most professionals and even normal people is whether it is destiny or lifestyle that makes us fat? Attempting to answer this query is a team from the Newcastle University, which has revealed that childhood genetic code could signify the chances of a person turning obese.

With many reports suggesting a range of health effects being passed on from the mother to the child, there is no question why obesity could not be one of the inherited factors. Influences of variables like depression, smoking, anti-depressant medications, BPA exposure and more during pregnancy are all cited to be carried forward to the unborn child.

"We were looking at the epigenetic patterns, the regulatory marks on genes. In this research we found a link between those found at birth and differences in body composition in children when they were nine. This suggests that our DNA could be marked before birth and these marks could predict our later body composition," shared Professor Caroline Relton, senior lecturer in epigenetic epidemiology at Newcastle University, lead author.

In terms of the present trial, the team located certain genes that manifested different sequences of expression in obese and normal weight children. This extensive study examined pregnant women and their children after birth. The kids were inspected for many years to understand how specific genes influenced their body compositions.

Though a relation between childhood obesity and genes during birth was established, the scientists asserted that further studies are required to affirm the findings. Experts essentially aimed to understand if changes in epigenetic factors could spur obesity.

Browse Archive